Limitations of Current Clinical Practice |
So why should we change our current practices? Our patients like us; our outcomes are generally good; we keep passing our board exams; and they keep telling us we have the best health care system in the world. Besides, change is hard work. Aren't we busy enough learning about how to survive in a managed care environment? Unfortunately, there is good evidence that the quality of care we give our patients could be better. Such evidence comes from:
In this section, we'll discuss each of these in turn. Practice without the best evidenceMuch of what we do is not supported by reliable external evidence, be it from randomized trials, systematic evaluation of a diagnostic test, or careful follow-up of large numbers of patients. Consider the following examples:
The failure of common senseThere are many, many more examples of "obvious" interventions which fail to help our patients live longer or better lives. How can common sense fail us so badly? As human beings, we are "wired" to respond to cues from the environment in certain, predictable ways. In particular, it has been very successful from an evolutionary perspective to look for causality in our environment:
Consider a similar example from the medical realm:
This "causality heuristic" is driven by our belief in ourselves as physicians, our belief in the value of pharmaceutical interventions (which have been so successful for other conditions), and our patients' desire to see their decision to visit the physician validated. A lawyer (or my high school Latin teacher) would say: post hoc ergo propter hoc, or "after this, therefore because of this". There are many other heuristics or "rules of thumb" which guide, and sometimes mislead, clinical decision-making. They have been described by Sox, and his book "Medical Decision Making" is an excellent source for further insights. For example, another common flaw in decision-making is the "availability heuristic". Certainly, we've all had the experience of diagnosing an unusual condition, then looking for it a little harder than usual in the next few patients. Physicians can also be led astray by ignoring the prior probability of disease. For example, the likelihood that chest pain is caused by coronary artery disease is vanishingly small in an otherwise healthy 20 year old, but may exceed 50% in an older patient with multiple risk factors. The youth may require only a careful history and reassurance, while for the older patient catheterization may be an appropriate initial study. While having a single diagnostic strategy for all patients with chest pain would be convenient, it would not be good practice. "Regression to the mean" is often unrecognized and can lead to inappropriate diagnoses and interventions. Consider a patient with a slightly elevated liver function test on the first measurement: Clearly, it is much more likely, just given random variation, that the second measurement will be lower than higher. There is much more of the curve to the left than to the right of the initial measurement! For another example, pretend the above numbers represent rookie batting averages. Any baseball fan can tell you about the "sophomore jinx": A player who has a great rookie season (hitting .320, for example) is unlikely to do as well or better their second year. That is regression to the mean at work. Variation in current practiceAs new tests and therapies are developed, how do physicians decide which to adopt? Without a clear, consistent framework, these decisions are typically driven by the practice patterns of local "opinion leaders", advertising, pharmaceutical representatives, specialists who may see a different spectrum of patients, and other potentially biased sources. The result is a huge variation in practice patterns among regions, states, and even cities in the same state.(Wennberg, 1991) Consider the following graph which shows the rate of radical prostatectomy per 100,000 male Medicare beneficiaries, adjusted for age and race (Lu-Yao, 1993): The range of surgery rates is incredible:
from 20/100,000 in Rhode Island to 429/100,000 in Alaska, a ratio
of 21 to 1! Since patients and prostates aren't that different
between those states, we must be considering different external
evidence. The result is that patients in some states undergo
too many prostatectomies, many of which leave the patient impotent
or incontinent. Conversely, patients in some states may
not be receiving the procedure often enough. The Managing medical informationEach month, thousands of medical journals publish tens of thousands of articles. Even if you only consider the 90 or so clinically oriented journals of most interest to primary care physicians, they publish over 15,000 articles per year. If you read 40 articles every day of the year, you would still fall woefully behind! Clearly, physicians can't read everything. In addition, much of the literature is in apparent conflict with a sort of "ping-pong" game played as issues are debated in the pages of journals. Results are often presented which are premature, use inappropriate outcomes, or represent communications to other researchers, not clinicians. Most importantly, many published studies suffer from serious flaws which invalidate their results. Unless specially trained in critical appraisal, physicians may be misled by these invalid results. A systematic, rational strategy is needed to deal with this information overload. Knowledge declines over timeAlthough medical knowledge declines as physicians get further from medical school (Sackett, 1997), their ability to practice the "art" of medicine improves. Physicians get to know their patients, hone their diagnostic skills, and are exposed to an ever increasing number of patients and problems. Wouldn't it be great if our medical knowledge also improved as we moved through our careers? In the next section, we will discuss how an evidence-based approach to practice, teaching, and research addresses these limitations of medical practice. |